TransWikia.com

Are /t, p, k/ aspirated when they are at the start of a syllable after another syllable that ends in /s/?

English Language & Usage Asked on May 11, 2021

In English (native speakers’ speech), voiceless plosives such as /t/, /p/ and /k/ are produced with a strong burst of air when they are in the start of a syllable before a vowel. That is called "aspiration" (represented by ʰ). For example,

  • kill [ɪl]
  • tar [ɑː(r)]
  • pie [aɪ]

But after a preceding /s/ in the same syllable, there is no aspiration:

  • skill [skɪl]
  • star [stɑː(r)]
  • spy [spaɪ]

Now there are words in English in which the /s/ is at the end of the first syllable and a voiceless plosive at the beginning of the next syllable:

  • whisper /ˈwɪs.pə(r)/
  • whiskey /ˈwɪs.ki/
  • sister /ˈsɪs.tə(r)/

It is clear that the syllable division is between the /s/ and /p/, /k/, /t/ in the above examples (from Cambridge Online Dictionary). The /p/, /k/ and /t/ are at the start of syllables and are expected to be aspirated since voiceless plosives are usually aspirated in that position. Nevertheless, whenever I aspirated those plosives, it feels like I’m overexaggerating.

My question is: are plosives at that position (in the start of a syllable after a syllable that ends with /s/) aspirated in native speakers’ speech?

2 Answers

Unfortunately, there is no definitive answer to a question about pronunciation in English because the pronunciation of any given word varies enormously from speaker to speaker. Moreover, syllabification is a highly controversial topic because of the following reasons:

  • there are many different approaches to syllabification in English
  • syllabification varies across idiolects1
  • there isn't a widely agreed on syllabification rule
  • most syllabification rules have lots of exceptions

Also, you say ‘voiceless plosives such as /t/, /p/ and /k/ are produced with a strong burst of air when they are in the start of a syllable before a vowel’; I don't agree entirely because for most speakers, it is at the start of a stressed syllable.

Words like whisper, sister and whiskey can be syllabified in many different ways. Some of the syllabification rules are:

Ambisyllabicity

Some linguists (such as Kahn2) think that the /s/ is ‘ambisyllabic’, meaning it belongs to both the preceding and the following syllable simultaneously. In his Syllable Structure: The Limits of Variation, San Duanmu has represented ambisyllabic consonants by underlining them. Other linguists represent them by transcribing them in {curly brackets}. I'll represent them by transcribing them separately with both the preceding and the following syllable for the sake of clarity:

  • whisper → [ˈwɪs.spə]
  • sister → [ˈsɪs.stə]
  • whiskey → [ˈwɪs.ski]

As the plosives are preceded by an /s/, they should be unaspirated, according to this analysis.

Maximal Onset Principle

According to MOP3, intervocalic consonants are syllabified as the onset of the following syllable as long as it doesn't violate the Phonotactic constraints. It would mean that VCV has to be syllabified as V.CV.

However, if you syllabify whisper, sister and whiskey as /ˈwɪ.spə/, /ˈsɪ.stə/ and /ˈwɪ.ski/ respectively, there's a violation of the Phonotactic constraints; stressed lax vowels—/ɪ, ʌ, ɒ, ʊ/—cannot occur syllable-finally i.e. there should be a consonant after those vowels, so whisper, sister and whiskey are syllabified as:

  • [ˈwɪs.pə]
  • [ˈwɪs.ki]
  • [ˈsɪs.tə]

According to this analysis, the plosives are syllable-initial which suggests that they should be aspirated; however, in practice, they are not aspirated as strongly as they would be in the beginning of a stressed syllable (cf. distaste, discomfort).

Wells' syllabification

Prof John Wells proposes a different syllabification theory4. He says that ‘consonants are syllabified with the more strongly stressed of two flanking syllables’. John Wells syllabifies5 whisper, sister and whiskey as, respectively:

  • [ˈwɪsp.ə]
  • [ˈsɪst.ə]
  • [ˈwɪsk.i]

He syllabifies the plosives with the coda of the first stressed syllables. And voiceless plosives in codas are usually unaspirated. So according to him, the [p, t, k] in the above examples are unaspirated.

Further observations

  • Some native speakers don't aspirate plosives at the start of a syllable following another syllable that ends in an /s/ so long as the the word is monomorphemic (having a single morpheme as in whiskey).
  • Some native speakers aspirate voiceless plosives across morpheme boundaries, for example, dis.taste.
  • Most, if not all, native speakers aspirate plosives across word boundaries as in this time
  • There are also some speakers that don't aspirate plosives across morpheme boundaries when preceded by an /s/ even if they're stressed, so the first /t/ in dis.taste might be pronounced without aspiration
  • Most speakers do not aspirate plosives in unstressed syllables

Notes and references

  1. Idiolect means ‘the language or speech pattern of one individual at a particular period of life’. [Merriam-Webster]
  2. Read this paper (25 - Ambisyllabicity in English: present and past) for Kahn's ambisyllabicity
  3. I've explained MOP in this answer to another question
  4. Syllabification and Allophony - UCL
  5. Those transcriptions are from Wells' Longman Pronunciation Dictionary

Correct answer by Decapitated Soul on May 11, 2021

The difference is between the 's' and the plosive consonant being parts of separate syllables and their being parts of a digraph representing a consonant phoneme.

The phoneme at the start of 'still' for example is not a combination of the phonemes represented by the letters 's' and 't' but is a separate phoneme which is represented conventionally by the digraph 'st'. It is possible to imagine an alphabet with a separate character for the phoneme 'st' but we don't have one in the 26-letter alphabet used in English so we use a conventional digraph instead,

Where there is a syllable boundary between the 's' and the plosive consonant the consonants merely happen to be adjacent and do not form a digraph, the second consonant is, therefore, pronounced normally with aspiration.

For an example of the above consider the words 'upstart' and 'pastime'. Upstart is equivalent to 'up' and 'start' so the 's' and 't' form a digraph representing a single phoneme and there is no separate 't' to be aspirated. On the other hand pastime is equivalent to 'pass' and 'time' so the 's' and 't' are part of separate syllables, do not form a digraph and represent separate phonemes so the 't' is aspirated.

Answered by BoldBen on May 11, 2021

Add your own answers!

Ask a Question

Get help from others!

© 2024 TransWikia.com. All rights reserved. Sites we Love: PCI Database, UKBizDB, Menu Kuliner, Sharing RPP